Proto-Munda language
Proto-Munda | |
---|---|
Reconstruction of | Munda languages |
Region | Mahanadi River Delta and adjacent coastal plains[1][2][3] |
Era | c. 2000 – c. 1500 BCE[4] |
Reconstructed ancestor | |
Lower-order reconstructions |
Proto-Munda is the reconstructed proto-language of the Munda languages of South Asia. It has been reconstructed by Sidwell & Rau (2015). According to Sidwell, the Proto-Munda language split from Proto- Austroasiatic in Indochina and arrived on the coast of Odisha around 4000 – 3500 years ago.[6]
Reconstruction
[edit]The following Proto-Munda lexical proto-forms have been reconstructed by Sidwell & Rau (2015: 319, 340-363).[7] Two asterisks are given to denote the tentative, preliminary state of the proto-language reconstruction.
Gloss | Proto-Munda |
---|---|
belly | **(sə)laɟ |
big | **məraŋ |
to bite | **kaˀp |
black | **kE(n)dE |
blood | **məjam |
bone | **ɟaːˀŋ |
to burn (vt.) | **gEˀp |
claw/nail | **rəmAj |
cloud | **tərIˀp |
cold | **raŋ |
die (of a person) | **gOˀj |
dog | **sOˀt |
to drink (water) | **uˀt, **uˀk |
dry (adj./stat.) | **(ə)sAr |
ear | **lutur, **luˀt |
earth/soil | **ʔOte |
to eat | **ɟOm |
egg | **(ə)tAˀp |
eye | **maˀt |
fat/grease/oil | **sunum |
feather | **bəlEˀt |
fire | **səŋal |
fish (n.) | **ka, **kadO(ŋ) |
fly (v.) | **pEr |
foot | **ɟəːˀŋ |
give | **ʔam |
hair (of head) | **suˀk |
hand | **tiːˀ |
to hear/listen | **ajɔm |
heart, liver | **(gə)rE, **ʔim |
horn | **dəraŋ |
I | **(n)iɲ |
to kill | **(bə)ɡOˀɟ |
leaf | **Olaːˀ |
to lie (down) | **gətiˀc |
long | **ɟəlƏŋ |
louse (head) | **siːˀ |
man/husband, person/human | **kOrOˀ |
meat/flesh | **ɟəlU(Uˀ) |
moon | **harkE, **aŋaj |
mountain/hill | **bəru(uˀ) |
mouth | **təmOˀt |
name | **ɲUm |
neck | **kO, **gOˀk |
new | **təmI |
night | **(m)ədiˀp |
nose | **muːˀ |
not | **əˀt |
one | **mOOˀj |
rain | **gəma |
red | **ɟəŋAˀt |
road, path | **kOrA |
root (of a tree) | **rEˀt |
sand | **kEˀt |
see | **(n)El |
sit | **kO |
skin | **usal |
sleep | **gətiˀc |
smoke (n.) | **mOˀk |
to speak, say | **sun, **gam, **kaj |
to stand | **tənaŋ, **tƏŋgə |
stone | **bərƏl, **sərEŋ |
sun | **siŋi(iˀ) |
tail | **pata |
thigh | **buluuˀ |
that (dist.) | **han |
this (prox.) | **En |
thou/you | **(n)Am |
tongue | **laːˀŋ |
tooth | **gənE |
tree | **ɟiːˀ |
two | **baːˀr |
to walk, go | **sEn |
to weave | **ta(aˀ)ɲ |
water | **daːˀk |
woman/wife | **selA, **kəni |
yellow | **saŋsaŋ |
Proto-Munda reconstruction has since been revised and improved by Rau (2019).
Morphosyntax
[edit]Syntactic shift
[edit]Although the modern Munda languages show a standard head-final subject–object–verb (SOV) order in unmarked phrases, most scholars believe that proto-Munda was head-first, VO like proto-Austroasiatic. The first linguist to noticed this peculiarity, Heinz-Jürgen Pinnow (1959, 1960), observed strong traceable evidence for a proto-Munda VO order. VO order has been found in compounds, noun incorporation verbal morphology in the Sora[8]-Gorum languages, and to a lesser extent in Gutob, Remo, Kharia, and Juang. By any given verb conjugations, the Munda verbs (including Kherwarian (Santali, Ho,...) and Korku) always show internal head-first, V-P order, with two main overall syntactic orders of transitive verbs: A-V-P and V-P-A, corresponding to Austroasiatic clausal syntaxes SVO and VOS. Most Munda compounds are also head-first and right-branching, with new loan words from Indian languages following the Indian norm of head-final and left-branching.[9][10]
Remo:
gui-ti
wash-hand
'wash hand'
Sora:
ɲen
1SG
dʒum-te-ti-n-ai
eat-banana-NPST-INTR-1SG.SUBJ
'I am eating banana'
Juang:
ba-ama-gito-ke
1DU.SUBJ-NEG-sing-PRES.TR
'We two don't sing'
Gorum:
ne-r-ab-so’ɟ-om
1SG.SUBJ-NEG-CAUS-learn-ACT:2SG.OBJ
'I didn’t teach you'
Winfred Lehmann (1973) reviewed,[11]
"If we examine further evidence provided by Pinnow, we note that Munda contains VO characteristics. It has VO order in compounds (Pinnow [1960], 97); it also provides examples of NG [noun-genitive] order and of prefixes. Since the Khmer-Nicobar languages are consistently VO, I assume that it was the Munda languages which were modified syntactically... We may conclude that Proto-Austroasiatic was VO and non-agglutinative in morphological structure."
— Lehmann, 1973:57
Pinnow (1963, 1966) proposed that proto-Munda was SVO and that was the syntax of proto-Austroasiatic, which was also highly synthetic like Munda, whereas he attributed analytic and isolating typological features in modern Mon-Khmer to language contact in the Mainland Southeast Asia linguistic area.[12] Disagreeing with Pinnow, Donegan & Stampe (1983, 2004) argued that proto-Munda was VO but non-agglutinative like its sister languages in Southeast Asia. According to Donegan & Stampe, Munda synthesis and SOV order were not stimulated by just language contact within the South Asian linguistic area, but by internal restructuring that caused the Munda word prosody to shift its rhythmic patterns from typical Austroasiatic rising, vowel reduction, iambic stressed to falling, vowel harmony, trochaic stressed profile, thus reversed the clausal syntactic structure and triggered word agglutination.[13]
For the reason why the Munda languages keep head-first order in compounds and polysynthetic morphology, Donegan & Stampe (2002) believed that words, like the verb-noun compounds, are more resistant to internal changes of rhythms and ordering than phrases.[14] Stanley Starosta (1967) explained that, during its early formation stage when Munda was still head-first SVO, verb-noun incorporation was facilitated as seen in modern Sora, but then it did a syntactic shift to head-final SOV and added more morphology. The polysynthetic verb phrases thus had become crystalized since that time. Sora verbs may incorporate another indirect object, but the theme argument would fall within the scope of different oblique arguments; the oblique noun is showed in thematic position. Sora also can incorporate transitive subjects and form verb serializations with incorporated nouns. Since the Sora NIs function like the mirror images of typical well-studied polysynthetic languages, it was suggested by Mark Baker that Sora simply operates the opposite way to the head movement theory proposed by the polysynthesis parameter.[15]
Donegan & Stampe's prediction of Munda synthetic shift caused by change of rhythmic holism is contested by Horo & Anderson et al. (2017, 2020, 2022). Field acoustic researches on various Sora and Santali dialects show that both languages have consistently second-or-last-syllable prominence with clear iambic patterns (first light syllable, followed by a second heavy stressed syllable) in disyllabic and tetrasyllabic words (Horo & Anderson 2022).[16][17]
Proto-Munda predicate
[edit]Anderson and Zide (2001, 2007)
[edit]Gregory D. S. Anderson & Norman Zide (2001, 2007) reconstructed the head-marking bound predicate of Proto-Munda with A-V-P order as following:
Slot | +4 | +3 | +2 | +1 | core | -1 | -2 | -3 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
role | SUBJ | NEG | RECIP/CAUS | DERIV | verb stem | PASS/INTR | TRANS/TNS | OBJ |
Rau (2020)
[edit]Felix Rau (2020) concludes that Proto-Munda predicate structure was verb-medial SVO,[18] though he suggests that it might have been less inflected with fewer bound elements, which may cause the Munda predicate development to become divergent later.[19]
Slot | +6 | +5 | +4 | +3 | +2 | +1 | core | -1 | -2 | -3 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
role | SUBJ | MOD/ASP | NEG | RECIP | CAUS | DERIV | verb stem | ASP | [other voices]/valency | OBJ |
reconstruction (Rau 2020:231) |
*A *O *Vj *mO |
*əˀt *Um |
*kƏl | *Oˀp | **bə- **tA- **A- |
*=lə Perf *=tə Imperf |
*n MID *ˀt ACT |
At some points during their early development in South Asia, due to either language contact led to adoptions of South Asian areal features or internal rhythmic changes, the Munda languages presumably made a syntactic shift from head-first, prefixing SVO to head-final, suffixing SOV.[20][21] Proto-North Munda restructured all prefixes and prepositions into suffixes. The situation is quite different in South Munda languages, especially Juang, Gtaʔ, and Sora-Gorum, where the original proto-Munda prefix slots are well-preserved, but later additional developments of their predicates are mostly suffixes or enclitics.[22]
Noun incorporation
[edit]According to Anderson (2014, 2017, 2021), Munda syntactic noun incorporation is very archaic and may be the oldest feature of Austroasiatic morphology, with cognates are attested in across every subgroup,[23] but the status of noun incorporation in proto-Munda still difficult to determine. Rau notes that "it is possible that some sort of incorporation was already present in proto-Munda and worked along the lines attested in modern Sora."[24]
See also
[edit]References
[edit]- ^ Rau, Felix; Sidwell, Paul (2019). "The Munda Maritime Hypothesis". Journal of the Southeast Asian Linguistics Society (JSEALS). 12 (2). hdl:10524/52454. ISSN 1836-6821.
- ^ Rau, Felix and Paul Sidwell 2019. "The Maritime Munda Hypothesis." ICAAL 8, Chiang Mai, Thailand, 29–31 August 2019. doi:10.5281/zenodo.3365316
- ^ Blench, Roger. 2019. The Munda maritime dispersal: when, where and what is the evidence?
- ^ Rau & Sidwell (2019:35)
- ^ Anderson (2020:160)
- ^ Sidwell, Paul. 2018. Austroasiatic Studies: state of the art in 2018 Archived 2019-05-03 at the Wayback Machine. Presentation at the Graduate Institute of Linguistics, National Tsing Hua University, Taiwan, 22 May 2018.
- ^ Sidwell, Paul and Felix Rau (2015). "Austroasiatic Comparative-Historical Reconstruction: An Overview." In Jenny, Mathias and Paul Sidwell, eds (2015). The Handbook of Austroasiatic Languages. Leiden: Brill.
- ^ Baker (1996:32)
- ^ Donegan & Stampe (2002:114–116)
- ^ Donegan & Stampe (2004:6)
- ^ Lehmann, Winfred P. (March 1973). "A Structural Principle of Language and Its Implications". Language. 49 (1). Linguistic Society of America: 47–66. doi:10.2307/412102. JSTOR 412102.
- ^ Donegan & Stampe (2004:7).
- ^ Donegan & Stampe (2004:26-27)
- ^ Donegan & Stampe (2002:116)
- ^ Baker (1996:33)
- ^ Horo, Luke; Anderson, Gregory D. S. (2021). "Towards a prosodic typology of Kherwarian Munda languages: Santali of Assam.". In Alves, Mark J.; Sidwell, Paul (eds.). Papers from the 30th Annual Meeting of the Southeast Asian Linguistics Society. JSEALS Special Publications 8. University of Hawai'i Press. pp. 298–317.
- ^ Horo, Luke; Anderson, Gregory D. S.; Singha, Aman; Sonowal, Ria Borah; Gomango, Opino (2023). "Acoustic phonetic properties of p-words and g-words in Sora". Proceedings of (Formal) Approaches to South Asian Linguistics 12: 144–167.
- ^ Rau (2020:227)
- ^ Rau (2020:225)
- ^ Rau (2020:228)
- ^ Anderson (2021:46)
- ^ Rau (2020:230)
- ^ Anderson (2021:37)
- ^ Rau (2020:209)
Further reading
[edit]- Alves, Mark J. (2020). "Morphology in Austroasiatic Languages". In Lieber, Rochelle (ed.). The Oxford Encyclopedia of Morphology. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.532.
- Anderson, Gregory D. S. (2007). The Munda verb: typological perspectives. Trends in linguistics. Vol. 174. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN 978-3-11-018965-0.
- Anderson, Gregory D. S., ed. (2008). Munda Languages. Routledge Language Family Series. Vol. 3. Routledge. ISBN 978-0-415-32890-6.
- Anderson, Gregory D. S. (2020). "Proto-Munda Prosody, Morphotactics and Morphosyntax in South Asian and Austroasiatic Contexts". In Jenny, Mathias; Sidwell, Paul; Alves, Mark (eds.). Austroasiatic Syntax in Areal and Diachronic Perspective. Brill Publishing. pp. 157–197. doi:10.1163/9789004425606_008.
- Anderson, Gregory D. S. (2021). "On the Role of Areal and Genetic Factors in the Development of the Word-Structure and Morphosyntax of the Munda Languages". In Mohan, Shailendra (ed.). Advances in Munda Linguistics. Cambridge Scholars Publishing. pp. 22–77. ISBN 1527570479.
- Baker, Mark C. (1996). The Polysynthesis Parameter. Oxford Studies in Comparative Syntax. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-509308-9. OCLC 31045692.
- Donegan, Patricia J.; Stampe, David (1983). "Rhythm and the holistic organization of linguistic structure". Chicago Linguistic Society. 19 (2): 337–353.
- Donegan, Patricia; Stampe, David (2002). "South-East Asian Features in the Munda Languages: Evidence for the Analytic-to-Synthetic Drift of Munda". In Chew, Patrick (ed.). Proceedings of the 28th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, Special Session on Tibeto-Burman and Southeast Asian Linguistics, in honour of Prof. James A. Matisoff. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society. pp. 111–129.
- Donegan, Patricia; Stampe, David (2004). "Rhythm and the Synthetic Drift of Munda". In Singh, Rajendra (ed.). The Yearbook of South Asian Languages and Linguistics. De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 3–36.
- Ghosh, Arun (2021). "Infixation in Munda and its Austroasiatic Legacy". In Mohan, Shailendra (ed.). Advances in Munda Linguistics. Cambridge Scholars Publishing. pp. 78–107. ISBN 1527570479.
- Rau, Felix (2020). "The Proto-Munda Predicate and the Austroasiatic Language Family". In Jenny, Mathias; Sidwell, Paul; Alves, Mark (eds.). Austroasiatic Syntax in Areal and Diachronic Perspective. Brill Publishing. pp. 198–235. doi:10.1163/9789004425606_009.